skip to main content
article

Personal space in virtual reality

Published:01 October 2006Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

Improving the sense of “presence” is a common goal of three-dimensional (3D) display technology for film, television, and virtual reality. However, there are instances in which 3D presentations may elicit unanticipated negative responses. For example, it is well established that violations of interpersonal space cause discomfort in real-world situations. Here we ask if people respond similarly when viewing life-sized stereoscopic images. Observers rated their level of comfort in response to animate and inanimate objects in live and virtual (stereoscopic projection) viewing conditions. Electrodermal activity was also recorded to monitor their physiological response to these stimuli. Observers exhibited significant negative reactions to violations of interpersonal space in stereoscopic 3D displays, which were equivalent to those experienced in the natural environment. These data have important implications for the creation of 3D media and the use of virtual reality systems.

References

  1. Adams, L. and Zuckerman, D. 1991. The effect of lighting conditions on personal-space requirements. Journal of General Psychology 118, 4, 335--340.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  2. Argyle, M. and Dean, J. 1965. Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry 28, 3, 289--304.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  3. Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., and Loomis, J. M. 2001. Equilibrium theory revisited: Mutual gaze and personal space in virtual environments. Presence-Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 10, 6, 583--598. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  4. Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., and Loomis, J. M. 2003. Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29, 7, 819--833.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  5. Barash, D. P. 1973. Human ethology---Personal space reiterated. Environment and Behavior 5, 1, 67--72.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. Bergman, B. A. 1971. The effects of group size, personal space, and success-failure upon physiological arousal, test performance, and questionnaire response. Dissertation Abstracts International 32, 6-A, 3419--3420.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  7. Blade, R. A. and Padgett, M. L. 2002. Virtual environments standards and terminology. In Handbook of Virtual Environments, Kay M. Stanney, Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ. 15--28.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  8. Critchley, H. D. 2002. Electrodermal responses: What happens in the brain. Neuroscientist 8, 2, 132--142.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  9. Diamond, R. and Carey, S. 1986. Why faces are and are not special---an efect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General 115, 2, 107--117.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  10. Durlach, N. and Slater, M. 2000. Presence in shared virtual environments and virtual togetherness. Presence-Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 9, 2, 214--217. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. Felipe, N. J. and Sommer, R. 1966. Invasions of personal space. Social Problems 14, 2, 206--214.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Garcia-Palacios, A., Hoffman, H., Carlin, A., Furness, T. A., and Botella, C. 2002. Virtual reality in the treatment of spider phobia: A controlled study. Behavior Research and Therapy 40, 9, 983--993.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. Hall, E. T. 1963. A system for the notation of proxemic behavior. American Anthropologist 65, 5, 1003--1026.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  14. Hill, K. J. and Howarth, P. A. 2000. Habituation to the side effects of immersion in a virtual environment. Displays 21, 1, 25--30.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  15. Holbrook, M. 1998. Breaking the camouflage: Stereography as the cure for confusion, clutter, crowding and complexity. PSA Journal 64, 30--36.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  16. Horton, D. and Wohl, R. R. 1956. Mass-communication and para-social interaction---observations on intimacy at a distance. Psychiatry 19, 3, 215--229.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  17. Kaufman, L. 1974. Sight and Mind: An Introduction to Visual Perception. Oxford University Press, New York.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  18. Lombard, M. 1995. Direct responses to people on the screen---television and personal-space. Communication Research 22, 3, 288--324.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  19. McBride, G., King, M. G., and James, J. W. 1965. Social proximity effects on galvanic skin responses in adult humans. Journal of Psychology 61, 1, 153.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. McLaughlin, M. L., Sukhatme, G., Peng, W., Zhu, W., and Parks, J. 2003. Performance and copresence in heterogeneous haptic collaboration. Proceedings of 11th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  21. Pertaub, D. P., Slater, M., and Barker, C. 2002. An experiment on public speaking anxiety in response to three different types of virtual audience. Presence-Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 11, 1, 68--78. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  22. Reeves, B. and Nass, C. 2000. Perceptual bandwith. Communications of the Acm 43, 3, 65--70. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  23. Reeves, B., Thorson, E., Rothschild, M. L., McDonald, D., Hirsch, J., and Goldstein, R. 1985. Attention to television---intrastimulus effects of movement and scene changes on alpha-variation over time. International Journal of Neuroscience 27, 3--4, 241--255.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. Short, J., Williams, E., and Christie, B. 1976. The Social Psychology of Telecommunications. Wiley, London.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  25. Slater, M. 2002. Presence and the sixth sense. Presence-Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 11, 4, 435--439. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. Slater, M. and Wilbur, S. 1997. A framework for immersive virtual environments (FIVE): Speculations on the role of presence in virtual environments. Presence-Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 6, 6, 603--616.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  27. Slater, M., Sadagic, A., Usoh, M., and Schroeder, R. 2000. Small-group behavior in a virtual and real environment: A comparative study. Presence-Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 9, 1, 37--51. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  28. Tromp, J., Bullock, A., Steed, A., Sadagic, A., Slater, M., and Frecon, E. 1998. Small group behavior experiments in the Coven project. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications 18, 6, 53--63. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  29. Yao, K. S., Matsui, T., Furukawa, H., Yao, T., Sakurai, T., and Mitsuyasu, T. 2002. A new stereoscopic endoscopy system: Accurate 3-dimensional measurement in vitro and in vivo with distortion-correction function. Gastrointestinal Endoscopy 55, 3, 412--420.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  30. Yee, N. 2005. The demographics, motivations, and derived experiences of users of massively multi-user online graphical environments. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments 15, 3, 309--329. Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Yin, R. K. 1969. Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology 81, 1, 141--151.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar

Index Terms

  1. Personal space in virtual reality

      Recommendations

      Comments

      Login options

      Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

      Sign in

      Full Access

      PDF Format

      View or Download as a PDF file.

      PDF

      eReader

      View online with eReader.

      eReader