skip to main content
research-article

The spatial resolution of crossmodal attention: Implications for the design of multimodal interfaces

Published:25 February 2009Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

Previous research on crossmodal attentional orienting has reported speeded reaction times (RT) when the stimuli from the different modalities are in the same spatial location and slowed RTs when the stimuli are presented in very different locations (e.g., opposite sides of the body). However, little is known about what occurs for spatial interactions between these two extremes. We systematically varied the separation between cues and targets to quantify the spatial distribution of crossmodal attention. The orthogonal cueing paradigm [Spence et al. 1998] was used. Visual targets presented above or below the forearm were preceded by either vibrotactile cues presented on the forearm, auditory cues presented below the forearm, or visual cues presented on the forearm. The presentation of both unimodal and crossmodal cues led to a roughly monotonic increase in RT as a function of the cue-target separation. Unimodal visual cueing resulted in an attentional focus that was significantly narrower than that produced by crossmodal cues: the distribution of visual attention for visual cues had roughly half of the lateral extent of that produced by tactile cueing and roughly one fourth of the lateral extent as that produced by auditory cueing. This occurred when both seven (Experiment 1) and three (Experiment 2) cue locations were used suggesting that the effects are not primarily due to differences in the ability to localize the cues. These findings suggest that the location of tactile and auditory warning signals does not have to be controlled as precisely as the location of visual warning signals to facilitate a response to the critical visual event.

References

  1. Chastain, G. 1992. Analog versus discrete shifts of attention across the visual field. Psych. Resear. 54, 175--181.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  2. Colonius, H. and Adndt, P. 2001. A two-stage model for visual-auditory interaction in saccadic latencies. Percep. Psychophy. 63, 126--147.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Diederich, A. and Colonius, H. 2007. Modeling spatial effects in visual-tactile saccadic reaction time. Percep. Psychophy.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  4. Diederich, A., Colonius, H., Bockhorst, D., and Tabeling, S. 2003. Visual-tactile spatial interaction in saccade generation. Exper. Brain Resear. 148, 328--337.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  5. Downing, C. J. and Pinker, S. 1985. The spatial structure of visual attention. In M. I. Posner and O. S. M. Martin, Eds. Attention and Performance XI. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 171--188.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  6. Driver, J. and Spence, C. 1998. Crossmodal links in spatial attention. Philosoph. Trans. the Royal Society Series B 353, 1319--1331.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  7. Edworthy, J. and Adams, A. 1996. Warning Design: A Research Prospective. Taylor & Francis, Bristol, PA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  8. Edworthy, J., Loxley, S., and Dennis, I. 1991. Improving auditory warning design: relationship between warning sound parameters and perceived urgency. Human Factors 33, 205--231.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  9. Eimer, M., and Van Velzen, J. 2005. Spatial tuning of tactile attention modulates visual processing within hemifields: an ERP investigation of crossmodal attention. Exper. Brain Resear. 166, 402--410.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  10. Farah, M. J., Wong, A. B., Monheit, M. A., and Morrow, L. A. 1989. Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? Neuropsychologica 27, 461--470.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  11. Ferris, T., Penfold, R., Hameed, S., and Sarter, N. 2006. The implications of crossmodal links in attention for the design of multimodal interfaces: a driving simulation study. In Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meeting on Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. HFES, Santa Monica, CA, 406--409.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Groh, J. M. and Sparks, D. L. 1996. Saccades to somatosensory targets II. Motor convergence in the primate superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiology 75, 428--438.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  13. Ho, C., and Spence, C. 2005a. Assessing the effectiveness of various auditory cues in capturing a driver's visual attention. J. Exper. Psych.: Applied 11, 157--174.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  14. Ho, C., Tan, H. Z., and Spence, C. 2005. Using spatial vibrotactile cues to direct visual attention in driving scenes. Transport. Resear. Part F: Traffic Psych. Behav. 8, 397--412.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. Ho, C., Tan, H. Z., and Spence, C. 2006. The differential effect of vibrotactile and auditory cues on visual spatial attention. Ergonomics 49, 724--738.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  16. Jones, C. M., Young, J. J., Gray, R., Spence, C., and Tan, H. Z. 2007. An eyetracker study of the haptic cuing of visual attention, peer reviewed extended abstract. In Proceedings of the World Haptics Conference (WHC07): The 2nd Joint EuroHaptics Conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. IEEE, Los Alamitos, CA. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  17. Kennett, S., Spence, C., and Driver, J. 2002. Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture. Percep. Psychophy. 64, 1083--1094.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. Rupert, A. H. 2000a. An instrumentation solution for reducing spatial disorientation mishaps—A more “natural” approach to maintaining spatial orientation. IEEE Engineering Med. Biology Mag. 19, 71--80.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Rupert, A. H. 2000b. Tactile situation awareness system: Proprioceptive prostheses for sensory deficiencies. Aviation Space Environ. Medicine 71, A92--A99.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. Shepherd, M. and Muller, H. J. 1989. Movement versus focusing of visual attention. Percept. Psychophy. 46, 146--154.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  21. Shulman, G. L., Remington, R. W., and McLean, J. P. 1979. Moving attention through physical space. J. Exper. Psych.-Hum. Percep. Perform. 5, 522--526.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. Shulman, G. L., Wilson, J., and Sheehey, J. B. 1985. Spatial determinants of the distribution of attention. Percep. Psychophy. 37, 59--65.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Spence, C. 2001. Crossmodal attentional capture: A controversy resolved? In C. Folk and B. Gibson, Eds. Attention, distraction and action: Multiple perspectives on attentional capture. Advances in Psychology, 133. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, 231--262.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  24. Spence, C. and Driver, J. 1996. Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. J. Exper. Psych. Hum. Percep. Perform. 22, 1005--1030.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  25. Spence, C. and Driver, J. 1997. Cross-modal links in attention between audition, vision, and touch: Implications for interface design. Int. J. Cognitive Ergonomics 1, 351--373.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  26. Spence, C. Nicholls, M. E. R., Gillespie, N., and Driver, J. 1998. Cross-modal links in exogenous covert spatial orienting between touch, audition, and vision. Percep. Psychophy. 60, 544--557.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. Spence, C., Pavaini, F., and Driver, J. 2000. Crossmodal links between vision and touch incovert endogenous spatial attention. J. Exper. Psych.-Hum. Percep. Perform. 26, 1298--1319.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Stein, B. M. and Meredith, M. A. 1993. The Merging of the Senses. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  29. Tan, H. Z., Gray, R., Young, J. J., and Traylor, R. 2003. A haptic back display for attentional and directional cueing. Haptics-e: Electron. J. Haptics Resear. 3.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  30. Wickens, C. D. 1980. The structure of attentional resources. In Nickerson, R. S., Ed. Attention and Performance VIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, 239--254.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Wiener, E. L. and Nagel, D. C. 1988. Human Factors in Aviation. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar

Index Terms

  1. The spatial resolution of crossmodal attention: Implications for the design of multimodal interfaces

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Sign in

    Full Access

    • Published in

      cover image ACM Transactions on Applied Perception
      ACM Transactions on Applied Perception  Volume 6, Issue 1
      February 2009
      117 pages
      ISSN:1544-3558
      EISSN:1544-3965
      DOI:10.1145/1462055
      Issue’s Table of Contents

      Copyright © 2009 ACM

      Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

      Publisher

      Association for Computing Machinery

      New York, NY, United States

      Publication History

      • Published: 25 February 2009
      • Accepted: 1 January 2008
      • Revised: 1 June 2007
      • Received: 1 January 2007
      Published in tap Volume 6, Issue 1

      Permissions

      Request permissions about this article.

      Request Permissions

      Check for updates

      Qualifiers

      • research-article
      • Research
      • Refereed

    PDF Format

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader