skip to main content
research-article

Investigating the performance of path-searching tasks in depth on multiview displays

Published:10 November 2010Publication History
Skip Abstract Section

Abstract

Multiview auto-stereoscopic displays support both stereopsis and head motion parallax depth cues and could be superior for certain tasks. Previous work suggests that a high viewpoint density (100 views/10cm at the eye) is required to convincingly support motion parallax. However, it remains unclear how viewpoint density affects task performance, and this factor is critical in determining display and system design requirements. Therefore, we present a simulated multiview display apparatus to undertake experiments using a path-searching task in which we control two independent variables: the stereoscopic depth and the viewpoint density. In the first experiment, we varied both cues and found that even small amounts of stereo depth (2cm) reliably improved task accuracy and reduced latency, whereas there was no evidence of dependence on viewpoint density. In the second experiment, we switched off the stereoscopic cue and varied viewpoint density alone. We found that for these monoscopic images increasing viewpoint density resulted in some reduction in response latency (up to eight views/10cm) but had no effect on accuracy. We conclude for cases where occlusion is not an overriding factor that low viewpoint densities may be sufficient to enable effective path-searching task performance.

References

  1. Bradshaw, M. F., Parton, A. D., and Glennerster, A. 2000. The task-dependent use of binocular disparity and motion parallax information. Vision Res. 40, 27, 3725--3734.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  2. Braunstein, M. L. 1968. Motion and texture as sources of slant information. J. Exp. Psychol. 78, 2, 247--253.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Bruno, N. and Cutting, J. E. 1988. Minimodularity and the perception of layout. J. Exp. Psych. Gen. 117, 2, 161--170.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  4. Dodgson, N. A. 2004. Variation and extrema of human interpupillary distance. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XI. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 36--46.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  5. Dosher, B. A., Sperling, G., and Wurst, S. 1986. Trade-offs between stereoscopic and proximity luminance covariance as determinants of perceived 3D structure. Vision Res. 26, 6, 973--990.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  6. Gilson, S. J., Fitzgibbon, A. W., and Glennerster, A. 2006. Quantitative analysis of accuracy of an inertial/acoustic 3D of tracking system in motion. J. Neurosci. Methods. 154, 1-2, 175--82.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  7. Glennerster, A., Rodgers, B. J., and Bradshaw, M. F. 1996. Stereoscopic depth constancy depends on the subject's task. Vision Res. 36, 21, 3441--3456.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  8. Hodges, L. F. and Mcallister, D. F. 1993. Stereo Computer Graphics and Other True 3D Technologies. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 71--89. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  9. Holliman, N. S. 2006. Handbook of Optoelectronics, vol. 2. Taylor & Francis, Boca Raton, FL.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  10. Hosokawa, K., Ohtsuka, S., and Sato, T. 2005. Depth perception from intermittent motion parallax stimuli. J. Vision 5, 8, 729--729.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  11. Hsu, J., Pizlo, Z., Chelberg, D., Babbs, C., and Delp, E. 1996. Issues in the design of studies to test the effectiveness of stereo imaging. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. Part A Syst. Humans 26, 6, 810--819. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  12. Johnston, E. B. 1991. Systematic distortions of shape from stereoscopic. Vision Res. 31, 7/8, 1351--1360.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. Johnston, E. B., Cumming, B. G., and Landy, M. S. 1994. Integration of stereoscopic and motion shape cues. Vision Res. 34, 17, 2259--2275.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  14. Jones, G. R., Lee, D., Holliman, N. S., and Ezra, D. 2001. Controlling perceived depth in stereoscopic images. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems VIII. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 42--53.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  15. Koenderink, J. J. and Van Doorn, A. J. 1991. Affine structure from motion. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 8, 2, 377--385.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  16. Lambooij, M. T. M., Ijsselsteijn, W. A., and Heynderickx, I. 2007. Visual discomfort in stereoscopic displays: A review. In Proceedings of the SPIE Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XIV. SPIE, Bellingham, WA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  17. Landy, M. S. and Kojima, H. 2001. Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. Opt. Image Sci. Vision 18, 9, 2307--2320.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. Landy, M. S., Maloney, L. T., Johnston, E. B., and Young, M. 1995. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion. Vision Res. 35, 3, 389--412.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  19. Mcallister, D. F. 1993a. Stereo Computer Graphics and Other True 3D Technologies. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  20. Mcallister, D. F. 1993b. Stereo Computer Graphics and Other True 3D Technologies. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 1--10. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  21. Merritt, J. O. and Cole, R. E. 1992. Interaction between binocular and monocular depth cues in teleoperation task performance. SID 92 Digest.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  22. Mitsuhashi, T. 1996. Evaluation of stereoscopic picture quality with ccf. Ergonomics 39, 11, 1244--1356.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Mon-Williams, M., Wann, J. P., and Rushton, S. 1993. Binocular vision in a virtual world: Visual deficits following the wearing of a head-mounted display. Ophthalmic. Physiol. Opt. 13, 4, 387--391.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  24. Nakanuma, H., Kamei, H., and Takaki, Y. 2005. Natural 3D display with 128 directional images used for human-engineering evaluation. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XII. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 28--35.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  25. Ogle, K. N. and Prangen, A. 1953. Observations on vertical divergences and hyperphorias. Arch Ophthal. 49, 313--334.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  26. Ono, M. E., Rivest, J., and Ono, H. 1986. Depth perception as a function of motion parallax and absolute distance information. J. Exp. Psych. Human Percept. Perform. 12, 331--337.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. Oruc, I., Maloney, L. T., and Landy, M. S. 2003. Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error. Vision Res. 43, 2451--2468.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Pastoor, S. and Schenke, K. 1989. Subjective assessments of the resolution of viewing directions in a multi-viewpoint 3D TV system. Proceedings of SID. 30, 3, 217--222.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  29. Peli, E. 1996. Health and safety issues with head-mounted displays(hmd). In Proceedings of the International Display Workshop. Society for Information Display, Campbell, CA, 493--496.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  30. Relke, I., Klippstein, M., and Riemann, B. 2004. Assessment and improvement of the stereo-image visualization on X3D technologies 3D displays. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XII. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 204--211.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Rogers, B. J. and Graham, M. E. 1979. Motion parallax as an independent cue for depth perception. Perception 8, 125--134.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  32. Runde, D. 2000. How to realize a natural image reproduction using stereoscopic displays with motion parallax. IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. Video Techn. 10, 3, 376--386. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  33. Sharma, K. and Abdul-Rahim, A. S. 1992. Vertical fusion amplitude in normal adults. Am J Ophthal. 114, 636--637.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  34. Siegel, M., Tobinaga, Y., and Akiya, T. 1999. Kinder gentler stereo. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems VI. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 18--27.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  35. Sollenberger, R. L. and Milgram, P. 1993. Effects of stereoscopic and rotational displays in a 3D path-tracing task. Human Factors. 35, 3, 483--499.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  36. Speranza, F., Tam, W. J., Martin, T., and Stelmach, L. 2005. Perceived smoothness of viewpoint transition in multi-viewpoint stereoscopic displays. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XII. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 72--82.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  37. Takeda, T., Hashimoto, K., Hiruma, N., and Fukui, Y. 1999. Characteristics of accommodation toward apparent depth. Vision Res. 39, 2087--2097.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  38. Tittle, J. S. and Braunstein, M. L. 1993. Recovery of 3D shape from binocular disparity and structure from motion. Percept. Psychophys. 54, 2, 157--169.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  39. Valyus, N. A. 1966. Stereoscopy. Focal Press, London.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  40. Ware, C. and Franck, G. 1996. Evaluating stereo and motion cues for visualizing information nets in three dimensions. ACM Trans. Graph. 15, 2, 121--140. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  41. Ware, C. AND Mitchell, P. 2005. Reevaluating stereo and motion cues for visualizing graphs in three dimensions. In Proceedings of the 2nd Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization. ACM, New York, 51--58. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  42. Wartell, Z. 2001. Stereoscopic head-tracked displays: analysis and development of display algorithms. Ph.D. thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  43. Williams, S. P. and Parrish, R. V. 1990. New computational control techniques and increased understanding for stereo 3D displays. In Proceedings of the Conference on Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XII. SPIE, Bellingham, WA, 73--82.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  44. Yeh, Y. and Silverstien, L. D. 1990. Limits of fusion and depth judgement in stereoscopic color displays. Human Factors 32, 1, 45--60. Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  45. Young, M. J., Landy, M. S., and Maloney, L. T. 1993. A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues. Vision Res. 33, 18, 2685--2696.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref

Index Terms

  1. Investigating the performance of path-searching tasks in depth on multiview displays

            Recommendations

            Comments

            Login options

            Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

            Sign in

            Full Access

            • Published in

              cover image ACM Transactions on Applied Perception
              ACM Transactions on Applied Perception  Volume 8, Issue 1
              October 2010
              156 pages
              ISSN:1544-3558
              EISSN:1544-3965
              DOI:10.1145/1857893
              Issue’s Table of Contents

              Copyright © 2010 ACM

              Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

              Publisher

              Association for Computing Machinery

              New York, NY, United States

              Publication History

              • Published: 10 November 2010
              • Accepted: 1 January 1996
              • Received: 1 February 1986
              Published in tap Volume 8, Issue 1

              Permissions

              Request permissions about this article.

              Request Permissions

              Check for updates

              Qualifiers

              • research-article
              • Research
              • Refereed

            PDF Format

            View or Download as a PDF file.

            PDF

            eReader

            View online with eReader.

            eReader